Summary

Advertisements for prescription drugs aim to increase use of the products. These advertisements make claims about the drugs, but also include non-propositional content.

The purpose of non-propositional content is to encourage good feelings about the products. This can be achieved with imagery that features happy people, fun activities or pleasing scenery.

Non-propositional content may lead health professionals to believe a drug is more beneficial or safer than the evidence suggests, even though they deny being influenced by advertising.

To avoid being misled by advertising, health professionals could analyse the claims being made and compare them with independent sources of drug information. Ignoring promotional material is another option.

 

Introduction

Direct-to-consumer advertising of prescription drugs is prohibited in Australia, however drugs are advertised to healthcare professionals. While perhaps not as contentious as direct-to-consumer advertising, promoting drugs to doctors is still controversial.

A common concern is that advertisers, who are paid to persuade, distort information to present a drug favourably. There is evidence that advertisements:

  • unduly emphasise benefits over harm1
  • fail to quantify serious risks2
  • make claims with inadequate or no substantiation3
  • rely heavily on research funded by the drug's manufacturer.4
 

Content of advertisements

The Competition and Consumer Act 2010 places broad constraints on the use of false or misleading representations in advertising.5 The Medicines Australia Code of Conduct also states that:

all information, claims and graphical representations provided to healthcare professionals … must be current, accurate, balanced and must not mislead either directly, by implication, or by omission.6

These restrictions mainly apply to the propositional content, which is explicit claims about the drug that can be assessed as true or false (see Box). This propositional content can be checked against independent resources and the product information, which refers to the drug's dose, indications, contraindications, precautions and adverse reactions.7 For example, an Australian advertisement for Circadin (prolonged-release melatonin) stated that it 'delivers quality restorative sleep with no evidence of rebound insomnia, tolerance, dependence or withdrawal symptoms. Patients awake refreshed and ready to face the day'.8 Each claim can, in principle, be verified or refuted by looking for supporting evidence.

Drug advertisements also contain non-propositional content, which persuades without conveying a proposition that can be proved true or false (see Box).9 Imagery is a commonly used form of non-propositional content. About half the Circadin advertisement was imagery. There were two pictures of a woman waking up. In the first, her head is barely raised and her eyes are half closed. The light is dim blue and the caption reads '7.00 am'. In the second, the woman is sitting up smiling and holding a cup of coffee. There is bright light and the caption reads '7.00 am: Circadin'. The background to both images is a dark sky that lightens as sun breaks through cloud.8 The implication may be that Circadin takes people out of the 'clouds' of insomnia into the 'light' of quality sleep. However, the picture is not just a didactic message. It aims to build positive emotional associations with the drug. To this end, many drug advertisements feature happy people, fun activities and majestic scenery. This non-propositional content is influential yet often escapes regulation because it is not obviously false or misleading.

 

Emotion and persuasion

Aristotle recognised the persuasive power of emotion in 'The art of rhetoric'. For Aristotle, to induce feeling or 'pathos' in the audience was as potent a rhetorical tool as the good character of the speaker ('ethos') and the clarity of their argument ('logos'). However, in 'Phaedrus', Plato cast emotion as an unruly horse threatening to upset the chariot of reason. The view of emotion as the enemy of reason was dominant during the Enlightenment period of history and persists widely today. Research, however, suggests that feeling or 'affect' is not only essential for decision making, but actually helps it.

Theorists now propose an 'affect heuristic' or shortcut by which feelings aid decisions.10 We unconsciously consider a range of options, tag each with feeling, and are biased to choose the one with the most positive affective reward. In short, we use feelings as information about what is good for us.11

Emotions may be especially useful when dealing with complex problems. People who rely on feelings to weigh many attributes of a new car may make better purchase decisions than others who engage in lengthy deliberation.12 In a gambling task, people have physiological changes when they are considering a choice that is risky, before they know it is a risky choice. Non-conscious biases guide their behaviour before their conscious knowledge does.13

Evaluative conditioning

Advertisers co-opt our feelings in ways that suit their persuasive goals. Evaluative conditioning is a prominent means by which branding creates positive feelings (see Box).14 Products for which we hold no special feeling are repeatedly paired with images or music that make us feel good. In a variant of classical conditioning, our good feelings eventually become linked to the product. Evaluative conditioning fosters more positive beliefs about a drug's safety and efficacy, and increases the intention to use it.15 This process occurs with little or no conscious awareness and the changes in belief are likely to persist.16

Consistent with drug advertising in the USA, print advertisements in Australian medical publications make copious use of imagery known to cause evaluative conditioning.9 Between 60%17 and 75%18 of US print direct-to-consumer advertising of prescription medicines includes an emotional appeal in the headline or visuals. Negative emotions such as fear, sadness or shame are associated with the illness or failure to use the drug. Positive emotions such as joy, happiness and humour often signal a return to normality through use of the product.

Emotional appeals are also created by the way promotional claims are worded. An advertisement for the antiplatelet drug ticagrelor urged readers to 'Save even more lives'.19 Positive words have been shown to produce evaluative conditioning,20 so this phrase is likely to persuade more than simply saying 'Reduce mortality', without being overtly misleading.

 

Are health professionals vulnerable to evaluative conditioning?

Many health professionals believe their specialised knowledge makes them less vulnerable to emotional persuasion. The 'elaboration likelihood' model gives some support to this view. It proposes that advertisements persuade by a central route that is conscious and deliberative, and by a peripheral route that is unconscious and automatic.21 (Note: this model does not refer to the central and peripheral nervous system.) Viewers are more likely to process information via the central route – the path taken by propositional content – if they have the motivation and ability to do so. Health professionals may be more motivated to focus on an advertisement's explicit claims and they clearly have more ability than a layperson to critique the claims.

The peripheral route of persuasion is the path taken by non-propositional content and is increased with distraction and time pressure. Busy doctors may be more vulnerable to this pathway than they think. A study found many doctors' prescriptions reflected commercial rather than academic sources of information, despite their denying the influence of advertising.22 Indeed, health professionals may mirror the wider community in being subject to the 'third person effect'. This is a psychological bias where others are deemed to be more easily persuaded by mass communication than oneself.23

Advertising in medical journals is effective. In the USA in 2005 drug companies spent just under half a billion US dollars on journal advertising.24 This returned approximately five dollars for every dollar invested.25 There is little doubt that the persuasive intent of drug advertising results in altered prescribing patterns.26

 

How can health professionals resist?

Two techniques for resisting evaluative conditioning have been analysed.27 'Persuasion knowledge priming' arms the viewer with an understanding that positive images bear little relation to a product's attributes and should be ignored. Alternatively, viewers are asked to relabel positive imagery with negative terms. Someone looking at the Circadin advertisement could, for example, try to ignore the smiling woman, or recite unpleasant adverse effects as they view her image. However, these techniques only marginally reduced conditioning in the cited study, and this studied beer rather than pharmaceuticals directly.27

Some researchers consider the peripheral route of persuasion to be 'mental contamination'.28 After reviewing a range of possible techniques, they conclude that reduced exposure to the persuasive stimulus is the only sure way to limit its influence. This means not looking at advertisements for drugs. If you do look at advertisements use an analytical approach. Be aware of the techniques that are being used to encourage prescribing of the product.

Using independent sources of information about drugs and therapeutics, such as Australian Prescriber, Australian Medicines Handbook and Therapeutic Guidelines may be beneficial. However, there is no evidence to show that independent sources can overcome the effects of evaluative conditioning.

 

How should regulators respond?

Regulating only propositional content may not reduce the influence of advertising. The US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) recognises the threat to accurate communication posed by non-propositional content in direct-to-consumer advertising of prescription medicines. In TV commercials it limits the use of distracting imagery when adverse effects are read. In print advertisements it bans 'signalling effects' where benefit information put in a headline has more impact than risks buried in the small print.29 The FDA is even conducting research on how direct-to-consumer advertising of prescription medicines impacts on unconscious or implicit attitudes.30

Calls to mandate statements of absolute risk in drug advertisements also recognise the power of non-propositional content. In particular, they aim to reduce so called 'framing effects' where the same information takes on different meaning when its format is varied. Stating a relative mortality reduction of, for example, 50% instead of an absolute reduction from 2% to 1%, does not promulgate a falsehood, but it does frame the information to present the treatment most favourably.31

 

Conclusion

Non-propositional content is effective and so it is unsurprising that positive imagery dominates many drug advertisements. If such content instils falsely favourable beliefs, it is doubtful that increased prescribing will benefit public health. Also, if the advertising of medicines to health professionals is to be properly regulated, research that quantifies the persuasive impact of non-propositional content is needed. Until then prescribers should be aware of the techniques of advertising and adopt an analytical approach when looking at advertisements for drugs.

Paul Biegler received funding from the Australian Research Council, specifically an Australian Postdoctoral Fellowship and a Discovery Project grant.

 
 

References

  1. Wilkes MS, Doblin BH, Shapiro MF. Pharmaceutical advertisements in leading medical journals: experts' assessments. Ann Intern Med 1992;116:912-9.
  2. Korenstein D, Keyhani S, Mendelson A, Ross JS. Adherence of pharmaceutical advertisements in medical journals to FDA guidelines and content for safe prescribing. PLoS One 2011;6:e23336.
  3. Santiago MG, Bucher HC, Nordmann AJ. Accuracy of drug advertisements in medical journals under new law regulating the marketing of pharmaceutical products in Switzerland. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak 2008;8:61.
  4. Cooper RJ, Schriger DL. The availability of references and the sponsorship of original research cited in pharmaceutical advertisements. CMAJ 2005;172:487-91.
  5. Australian Government ComLaw. Competition and Consumer Act 2010. www.comlaw.gov.au/Details/C2011C00003/Html/Volume_1#param7 [cited 2014 Sep 1]
  6. Medicines Australia. Code of Conduct. Edition 17. www. medicinesaustralia.com.au
  7. Australian Government Department of Health. Therapeutic Goods Administration. Guidance 8: Product Information. 2013. www.tga.gov.au/industry/pm-argpm-guidance-8-00.htm [cited 2014 Sep 1]
  8. Aspen Australia. Advertisement for Circadin. Aust Fam Physician 2013;42(9). p. 615.
  9. Biegler P, Vargas P. Ban the sunset? Nonpropositional content and regulation of pharmaceutical advertising. Am J Bioeth 2013;13:3-13.
  10. Slovic P, Finucane M, Peters E, MacGregor DG. The affect heuristic. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D, editors. Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002.
  11. Schwarz N, Clore GL. How do I feel about it? The informative function of affective states. In: Fiedler K, Forgas JP, editors. Affect, cognition and social behavior: new evidence and integrative attempts. Toronto: Hogrefe Publishing; 1988.
  12. Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, van Baaren RB. On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science 2006;311:1005-7.
  13. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 1997;275:1293-5.
  14. Hofmann W, De Houwer J, Perugini M, Baeyens F, Crombez G. Evaluative conditioning in humans: a meta analysis. Psychol Bull 2010;136:390-421.
  15. Biegler P, Kennett J, Oakley J, Vargas P. Ethics of implicit persuasion in pharmaceutical advertising. In: Clausen J, Levy N, editors. Handbook of Neuroethics. Dordrecht: Springer. Forthcoming 2015.
  16. Vansteenwegen D, Francken G, Vervliet B, De Clercq A, Eelen P. Resistance to extinction in evaluative conditioning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2006;32:71-9.
  17. Woloshin S, Schwartz LM, Tremmel J, Welch HG. Direct-to consumer advertisements for prescription drugs: what are Americans being sold? Lancet 2001;358:1141-6.
  18. Huhmann BA, Argo JJ, Main KJ. Pharmaceutical advertising in the USA: information or influence? Int J Advert 2004;23:119-42.
  19. AstraZeneca. Advertisement for Brilinta. Aust Fam Physician 2013;42(10).
  20. De Houwer J, Baeyens F, Eelen P. Verbal evaluative conditioning with undetected US presentations. Behav Res Ther 1994;32:629-33
  21. Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 1986;19:123-205.
  22. Avorn J, Chen M, Hartley R. Scientific versus commercial sources of influence on the prescribing behavior of physicians. Am J Med 1982;73:4-8.
  23. Davison WP. The third-person effect in communication. Public Opin Q 1983;47:1-15.
  24. Lexchin J. Analysing pharmaceutical advertisements in medical journals. In: Mintzes B, Mangin D, Hayes L, editors. Understanding and responding to pharmaceutical promotion: a practical guide. 1st ed. Geneva: World Health Organization/Health Action International; 2009. p. 41-60.
  25. Fugh-Berman A, Alladin K, Chow J. Advertising in medical journals: should current practices change? PLoS Med 2006;3:e130.
  26. Spurling GK, Mansfield PR, Montgomery BD, Lexchin J, Doust J, Othman N, et al. Information from pharmaceutical companies and the quality, quantity, and cost of physicians' prescribing: a systematic review. PLoS Med 2010;7:e1000352.
  27. Sweldens S, Van Osselaer SMJ, Janiszewski C. Evaluative conditioning procedures and the resilience of conditioned brand attitudes. J Consum Res 2010;37:473-89.
  28. Wilson TD, Centerbar DB, Brekke N. Mental contamination and the debiasing problem. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D, editors. Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002.
  29. U.S. Food and Drug Administration. Guidance for industry: presenting risk information in prescription drug and medical device promotion. Rockville, MD: FDA; 2009.
  30. U.S. Food and Drug Administration. A supplementary test of distraction in DTC advertising using an implicit measure, the affect misattribution procedure. Rockville, MD: Center for Drug Evaluation and Research, FDA; 2011.
  31. Malenka DJ, Baron JA, Johansen S, Wahrenberger JW, Ross JM. The framing effect of relative and absolute risk. J Gen Intern Med 1993;8:543-8.

Paul Biegler

Adjunct research fellow, Centre for Human Bioethics School of Philosophical, Historical and International Studies, Faculty of Arts, Monash University, Melbourne